Top Qs
Timeline
Chat
Perspective

Kulina language

Arawan language of Brazil and Peru From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Remove ads

Kulina (also Kulína, Kulyna, Culina, Curina, Corina, Korina, Culina-Madijá, Madijá, Madija, Madiha, Madihá) is an Arawan language of Brazil and Peru spoken by about 4,000 Kulina people. With such few speakers, Kulina is considered a threatened language.

Quick Facts Culina, Native to ...
Remove ads

History and geography

The Kulina people traditionally live in the states of Acre and Amazonas in Brazil and the Ucayali region in Peru.[2] In Acre and Ucayali, the villages are found along the Purus and Envira rivers. In Amazonas, the villages are around the Juruá, Tarauacá and Jutaí rivers.[2]

Classification

Kulina is a member of the Arawan language family.[2] According to Dienst (2014), it forms a Madihá dialect continuum with Western Jamamadi and Deni.[2] The term madihá means 'people' in all of these languages.[2]

Kulina is similar to the Deni language, as they have even been considered different dialects of the same language. Both languages have SOV word order,[3] as well as three sets of alveolar affricate consonants. It is believed the presence of the reconstructed phoneme *s in place of the fricative *sh is indicative of the Kulina and Deni languages as opposed to other languages in the Arawan family.[4]

Remove ads

Phonology

Consonants

More information Labial, Dental ...

Consonants sounds /pʰ, t̪, d͡z, t͡s, t͡sʰ, ɾ, β~w/ may also be pronounced as /ɸ~f, t͡ʃ, z~ɟ, s, sʰ~ʃ, l, v/.

Vowels

More information Front, Back ...

An [a] sound can also range to a [ɨ] sound. The [u] vowel sound only appears in diphthongs.[2]

Grammar

The basic constituent order is subject–object–verb. It is predominantly a head-marking language with agglutinative morphology and some fusion.[2] Kulina is a head-final language and contains many more suffixes than prefixes.[2] There are two noun classes and two genders and agreement on transitive verbs is determined by a number of complex factors, both syntactic and pragmatic.[2] In transitive sentences, the verb agree with the object in gender and with the subject in person and number. In intransitive sentences, the verb agrees with its subject in person, number and gender.[2]

Remove ads

Bibliography

  • Boyer, Cindy & Jim Boyer. 2000. Dictionario: (sic) Culina - Castellano. Unpublished Manuscript.
  • Dienst, Stefan. 2014. A Grammar of Kulina. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
  • Dienst, Stefan. 2009. "Stative Verbs in Kulina". ReVel Revista Virtual de Estudos de Lingaugem.
  • Diesnt, Stefan. 2008a. "Portuguese Influence on Kulina". In Thomas Stolz, Dik Bakker & Rosa Salas Palomo (eds.) Aspects of language contact. New theoretical, methodological and empirical findings with special focus on Romancisation processes, 287-297. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Dienst, Stefan. 2008b. "Why Kulina doesn't have an antipassive". Amerindia 32: 27-36.
  • Dienst, Stefan. 2005. "The innovation of s in Kulina and Deni". Anthropological Linguistics 52: 209-243.
  • Monserrat, Ruth Maria Fonini & Abel O. Silva. 1986. Gramática da língua Kulina. Dialeto do Igarapé do Anjo. Acre: Conselho Indigenista Missionário.
  • Silva, Abel O. & Ruth M. F. Monserrat.1984. Dicionário Kulina – Português e Português – Kulina. (Dialeto do Igarapé do Anjo). Acre: Conselho Indigenista Missionário.
  • Tiss, Frank. 2004. Gramática da língua Madiha (Kulina). São Leopoldo: Oikos.
  • Wright, Pamela Sue. 1991. La hipótesis del inacusativo y los verbos mádija (culina). Revista Latino-americana de Estudios Ethnolingüísticos 6: 49-62.
  • Wright, Pamela Sue. Madija predicates. Working Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota 39: 93-140.
Remove ads

References

Loading related searches...

Wikiwand - on

Seamless Wikipedia browsing. On steroids.

Remove ads