Top Qs
Timeline
Chat
Perspective

Menya language

Language From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Remove ads

Menya (Menyama, Menye) is an Angan language of Papua New Guinea.

Quick Facts Native to, Region ...

Classification

Menya is classified as an Angan language. Its closest relatives in terms of shared vocabulary are Kapau and Yagwoia at 69-75% and 50-58% respectively.[2] As the Menya-speaking area is bordered on all sides by speakers of other Angan languages, much of the Menya grammar and vocabulary has remained intact and is consistent with other Angan languages. Most ethnographic evidence suggest that the Menya-speaking people do not think of themselves as culturally distinct from their neighbors, as they have no words for Menya identity and people.[2]

Remove ads

Status

Menya is used by the community by all age groups, although there is a growing bilingualism and use of Tok Pisin, and to a lesser extent, English. The Menya language is now being shaped by this bilingualism, as shifts in vocabulary and grammar have become apparent. Nonetheless, unlike in other areas of New Guinea, language attitudes towards Menya remain positive, and the language is not endangered.[2]

Remove ads

Phonology

Consonants

The language is notable for its lack of fricative phonemes (other than the glottal /h/) and liquid phonemes.

More information Labial, Dental ...

Fricatives most often appear as realizations of plosives between vowels. Similarly, the liquids [ɾ] and [l] are allophones of /t̪/ in intervocalic position.

It is also worth noting the uvular plosive is reported to be the most frequently occurring phoneme in the language.

Menya's phonotactic constraints generally prohibit certain consonant clusters; as a result, an epenthetic vowel is frequently inserted to break up such clusters. Additionally, word-final consonants are often followed by a vocalic release; that is, /p/ tends to be followed by [u], while both /t/ and /k/ release to [i].

Vowels

More information Front, Central ...

Morphosyntax

Summarize
Perspective

The basic clause order of Menya is SOV. The language is dominantly right-headed, with suffixes and enclitics being far more prominent than prefixes and proclitics.

Menya sentences require one of the following clitics to express the speaker's motivation:

More information =i ~ =nji, =ta ...

Since verbs usually come last in Menya sentences, that's where clitics are often appended. However, this is not always the case.

If the last word of the sentence is a noun, an adjective or a verb, the clitic is =i.

Iqueqä häwqa, quâuqe.

i=qu=e=qä

that=M=OBJ=POSS

häwqa

tail

quäuqä=i

long=IND

i=qu=e=qä häwqa quäuqä=i

that=M=OBJ=POSS tail long=IND

Its tail is long.

Nyi yŋŋä kukŋuä ätäqä iqu, iqueqä yoqe haquŋui.

nyi

1S

yŋŋä

bird

kukŋuä

speech

ä-t-qäqä

ASSERT-say-PFV-1S/DSO

i=qu

that=M

i=qu=e=qä

that=M=OBJ=POSS

yoqä=i

name=DEF

haquŋuä=i

haquŋuä=IND

nyi yŋŋä kukŋuä ä-t-qäqä i=qu i=qu=e=qä yoqä=i haquŋuä=i

1S bird speech ASSERT-say-PFV-1S/DSO that=M that=M=OBJ=POSS name=DEF haquŋuä=IND

The bird I've talked about, its name is haquŋuä.

Otherwise, the clitic is =nji:

Ŋqä aŋi mäŋinji.

n=qä

1S=POSS

aŋä=i

house=DEF

m=ŋi=nji

down=LOC=IND

n=qä aŋä=i m=ŋi=nji

1S=POSS house=DEF down=LOC=IND

My house is down there.

Polar questions, marked with the =ta clitic, are questions which can be answered with either of two choices. They usually come down to a “yes” or “no”:

Matinäktanä?

Matinä=k=ta=nä

Martin=2S=PQ=QUOT

Matinä=k=ta=nä

Martin=2S=PQ=QUOT

“Are you Martin?”

However, those two alternatives can be anything:

Eqä asique, hŋque mända?

eqä

water

ak=qu=e

same=M=OBJ

hŋ=qu=e

INDEF=M=OBJ

mända

beyond

eqä ak=qu=e hŋ=qu=e mända

water same=M=OBJ INDEF=M=OBJ beyond

Was it the same river or one beyond?

Content questions, or wh–questions, are most frequently marked with the clitic =wä, but in some cases, one may come across the indicative =i or the much more limited clitic =kä. Content questions are characterized by the fact that they do not limit the answer to a binary choice.

An example of a content question is:

Qe äŋginyqäwä?

qe

2D

äŋgi=ŋqä-wä

where=GT=WH.Q

qe äŋgi=ŋqä-wä

2D where=GT=WH.Q

Where are you two (going)?

Dubitative mood constructions use the clitic =ti. They express a speaker's lack of knowledge or certainty on a given topic.

Iqu ämaqä naqäquti.

i=qu

that=M

ämaqä

person

naqä=qu=ti

big=M=DUB

i=qu ämaqä naqä=qu=ti

that=M person big=M=DUB

Is he an important person, I wonder.

Aŋi täqueqäti.

aŋä=i

house=that

tä=qu=e=qä=ti

this=M=OBJ=POSS=DUB

aŋä=i tä=qu=e=qä=ti

house=that this=M=OBJ=POSS=DUB

I wonder (I do not know) whose house this is.

Verbs

Verbs are bound forms, meaning they cannot occur in isolation. Morphologically, they are the most complex word class.

A verb may be marked by two sets of prefixes and up to seven suffixes. The prefixes indicate the polarity of assertion and, in the case of most transitive verbs, encode the person and number of the affected animate referent. The suffixes convey a range of grammatical information, including valency, tense, aspect, modality, the person and number of the actor, as well as relational affixes that connect the verb to the following clause.

Menya exhibits verb serialization, where two verb roots are compounded to form a complex stem. This process is limited in that only up to two verbs can be combined, and the second verb must be a verb of motion. See the example below:

…ne quea qe ätqämanmiŋque.

ne

1P

quea

story

qe

CERT

ä-t-qäma-n-miŋ-qäqu=i

ASSERT-say-remain-DETR-PST/IPFV-1P/DSO=IND

ne quea qe ä-t-qäma-n-miŋ-qäqu=i

1P story CERT ASSERT-say-remain-DETR-PST/IPFV-1P/DSO=IND

…we remained there telling stories.

One type of verbs, the so-called final verbs, are forms inflected for tense, past or present. They occur primarily in the last clause of a sentence, where the mood clitic is appended. There are three degrees of past tense: near past (within 2–3 days), past (up to 10 years ago), and remote past (more than 10 years ago). Furthermore, the primary aspect distinction is perfective (event as a whole) vs. imperfective (focus on internal complexity). Imperfective can be further subdivided into progressive (developing/repeated through time) and stative (continuing without change)

Another subcategory of verbs is that of medial verbs. These are non-finite forms—that is, lacking tense and mood—that indicate the relationship between clauses and whether the topical entity of the marked clause is the same or different from the following clause.

Noun phrases

Pronouns

Pronouns can be divided into three types: personal pronouns, demonstrative pronouns, and dyadic kin pronouns. Moreover, all nouns and pronouns can be the head of a phrase.

Personal pronouns, distinguish between the singular, dual, and plural numbers, as well as the first, second, and third persons. However, Menya does not differentiate between grammatical genders or make inclusive–exclusive distinctions in pronouns.

More information singular, dual ...

Speakers generally prefer to use demonstrative pronouns rather than the third-person singular ki. As for the third dual form, it has not been observed in the unmarked form, suggesting that it may be an extended use of the second person dual (“you two”).

In addition, these basic (i.e. unmarked) personal pronouns, there also exist two genitive and one emphatic form. Below is a table of all these forms along with their underlying composition.

More information singular, dual ...

Despite no discernible difference in native speaker evaluation or general usage, =qä forms are significantly more frequent than =yaqä forms, especially outside the first person.

Genitive pronouns are used to show possession. These words can either function as the pre-head possessor within a noun phrase or stand on their own.

…neqä aŋämäqä qaŋä qe äwäkuqe.

ne=qä

1P=POSS

aŋä=m=ngqä

house=unseen=GT

qaŋä

walk

qe

CERT

ä-wä-k-qäqu=i

ASSERT-go.down-PST/PFV-1P/DSO=IND

ne=qä aŋä=m=ngqä qaŋä qe ä-wä-k-qäqu=i

1P=POSS house=unseen=GT walk CERT ASSERT-go.down-PST/PFV-1P/DSO=IND

…we walked down to our far away home(s).

The house over there is mine.

aŋä

house

n=ŋi

level=LOC

n=yaqä=i

1S=POSS=IND

aŋä n=ŋi n=yaqä=i

house level=LOC 1S=POSS=IND

Aŋä näŋi, nyaqe.

Seven demonstrative pronouns can be distinguished:

More information near, far ...

Forms highlighted in blue are free morphemes, meaning they can stand alone. The remaining forms are bound morphemes and always need a clitic appended. When referring to humans, demonstratives usually take enclitics that can further encode class. These classes are: masculine (=qu), feminine (=i ~ =u), honorific (=pa), and diminutive (=pu ~ =su). The masculine is considered the default form in that it is used not only for male referents but also when gender is irrelevant. Honorific forms are mainly used when referring to older people, and the diminutive — to children.

Only subject noun phrases use unmarked demonstrative pronouns; personal pronouns, however, can serve as both subject and object arguments.

Dyadic kin pronouns combine specific familial relationship and therefore occur only dual and plural forms. They are constructed by combining a personal or demonstrative pronoun, a kin pronoun root that defines the relationship, and an appropriate person–number clitic.

Nouns

The category of nouns is the most extensive. It can be divided into three sub-categories: simple, complex, and kin.

Basic nouns are single, monomorphemic units that denote a wide variety of entities, such as abstract concepts, concrete objects, and both common and proper names.

Complex nouns fall into three main groups: two-root compounds, verbal derivatives, and those combining a bound root with a seemingly archaic noun class suffix.

Although noun compounding was once a productive process, it is no longer active.

yämbuayä (“manioc”) ← (“tree) + buayä (“sweet potato”)
bequwäqä (“sweet potato leaf”) ← buayä + quwäqä (“leaf”)
yäquwqä (“leaf of a tree”) ← + quwqä (“leaf”)

Additionally, nouns can be formed from verbs using the –qä suffix. It's common to see this suffix used to derive a noun phrase's head from a verbal phrase or even a full clause.

Kinship terms in this language often show significant irregularities due to suppletion or allomorphic variation. For instance, the word for “my father” or “our father” uses a completely different root (ap–) than the word for "your father" or "his/her father," which uses the root −n− with a prefix to indicate the possessor. This means there's rarely a need for a first-person prefix, as the root itself changes to show possession by the first person.

Not all kin terms, however, fall into the subclass of kin nouns; indeed, some of the most frequently encountered kin relationships are denoted by basic nouns or phrases:

qokä: “man, male, husband”
apäkä: “woman, female, wife”

Noun phrases are capable of incorporating various elements such as adjectives (which permit the intensifier –näŋä), adpositional phrases, quantifiers, demonstratives, possessives, personalizing clitics, and ellipses.

Menya has multiple case-marking clitics, also known as role markers. These can indicate: time, location, goal, source, and association (comitative).

Modifiers

Adverbs and adjectives are a vague class. It isn't clear whether they are separate classes, subclasses, or one class with semantically restricted usage of the words therein. For example, the word tnäŋä “hot, fast” can modify either nouns, like adjectives do, or verbs, like adverbs.

In noun phrases, modification generally follows the head noun, which is a deviation from Menya's dominant right-headed pattern (cf. genitives, which precede their head). In verbal phrases, however, the modifier comes before the head (the verb).

Nevertheless, the noun head can be omitted, leaving a modifier in the role of the phrase's head. Because of this, nouns could also be categorized as belonging to the same class as noun modifiers.

Remove ads

Bibliography

  • Whitehead, Carl R. (March 2006). A reference grammar of Menya, an Angan language of Papua New Guinea (PDF) (Ph.D.). University of Manitoba. Retrieved 17 July 2025.

References

Loading related searches...

Wikiwand - on

Seamless Wikipedia browsing. On steroids.

Remove ads